1. Introduction
Electroencephalogram (EEG) is a powerful tool used for studying cortical excitability during motor performance [
1,
2]. Attention is essential in motor skills preparation, because attentional shift is important in elite athletes and competitive situations [
3]. Sensorimotor Rhythm (SMR) is used to examine differences in cortical activities. It is also an indicator of attention [
4]. Moreover, it is inversely associated with sensorimotor activity of the cortex. SMR training investigations indicate that people can increase the power of SMR to maintain accuracy and attention in performance [
5]. Therefore, researchers should design neurofeedback trainings that can enhance the activity of SMR and to facilitate motor performance in individuals. SMR training increases the working memory capacity and the focus of attention, which improves the preparation of motor action [
6] and elevates the mood [
7].
Expert athletes show more particular cortex activities than novices during planning and performing movements [
8]. For example, expert athletes show reduced verbal-analytical processes in left temporal regions [
9] and increased SMR power at Cz4 [
10]. Cheng et al. examined the SMR differences in novices and expert dart players. Their results demonstrated a higher SMR power before the throw in skilled players [
4]. Therefore, the motor task can be performed more accurately by increasing the SMR power waves. Thus, the motor perception decreases following an increase in SMR activity, to develop focus and relaxation as a result of the diminishing processes of the somatosensory elements. Furthermore, Doppelmayr and Weber discovered that besides significantly increasing the SMR amplitude, the augmented SMR training positively affects the spatial-rotation ability and decision-making reaction time [
11]. The present study aimed to examine whether various amounts of SMR power waves are required when performing bimanual coordination activities in either of the 2 opposite phases.
An appropriate performance of bimanual movements is constrained by essential principles of coordinative behaviors
[12-16]. The relative phases of 00 and 1800 in the in-phase and anti-phase, create stable coordination patterns, respectively, in terms of the upper limb in the horizontal plane [
14-16]. The in-phase coordination mode involves simultaneous activation of the homologous group of muscles by concurrent mirror symmetric movements away and towards the body through the body centerline [
14,
17,
18]. Simultaneous movements from one side of the body to the other through the midline of the body produce iso-directional (parallel) patterns of movements in the natural space that generate the anti-phase coordination mode [
17,
19,
20]. The in-phase coordination does not change after an increase in the frequency of movement, while the anti-phase coordination becomes unstable. An eventual spontaneous transition is achieved in the in-phase, if the increasing frequency is not opposed [
12,
21,
22].
Kelso suggested a task in both fixed patterns including the 00-relative phase and 1800-relative phase, to study the movements of 2 limbs. Increasing the motion speed significantly affects the anti-phase [
12,
21]. However, the model lacks observable effects on the in-phase design. The anti-phase pattern variably corrupts, changes and reduces its precision, by the gradual increase of rate or motion [
13,
23]. This finding suggests that the in-phase does not readily change, compared to the opposite patterns [
17,
24]. In other words, the required attention process is more extensive in the anti-phase than in-phase patterns. We employed a task in the bimanual coordination of the 00-phase and the 1800-phase models using movements of flexion and the extensions. The study participants used visual, proprioception or audition feedback loops to extensively control the limbs, in order to investigate the continuous nature of the bimanual actions.
EEG studies strongly suggest that cortical activity, related to the band-limited EEG analysis, demonstrate the cognitive processes with remarkable functionality and provide more information than wide EEG analysis [
25,
26]. Therefore, we examined the waves separately in the present study. However, the question raised based on the presented material is whether the SMR power differs between the anti-phase and in-phase patterns. Also which coordination patterns requires more SMR power, in case of any differences. In addition, we compared SMR power at different speeds of each model. This designed experiment also investigated the changes in the performance and stability of bimanual coordination models at varying movement speeds. This study also explained how they differ from each other with respect to the degrees of SMR power.
2. Materials and Methods
This was a quasi-experimental study with factorial design. The statistical population included all non-physical education major students of Shahid Beheshti University. Study participants were selected using convenience sampling method. All participants were right-handed (according to E.H. Inventory; Oldfield) [
27]. The samples aged between 19 and 24 years with the mean age of 21 years. They were included according to standardized self-report auditions, the Snellen chart test, and lack of any diagnosed neuromuscular, motor and/or sensory disorders. The health conditions of the participants were investigated using a questionnaire. These questionnaires were completed prior to obtaining the informed consent forms, in order to assess the subjects with regard to the inclusion criteria of the study. The findings of present study could be applicable for improving the motor performance in healthy young males by means of SMR neurofeedback training.
Study procedure
The ProComp Infiniti (2180 Belgrave Avenue, Montreal, QC H4A 2L8 Canada) encoder is a computerized biofeedback data acquisition device with 8 multi-modal channels for real-time recording. It has 8 pin sensor inputs and 2048 s/s and 256 s/s samples of 2 and 6 channels, respectively. The ProComp Infiniti encoder can objectively read a broad scope of clinical and physiological signs for observation and biofeedback. We reorganized functionality applying BioGraph software.
Only wrist movements of the flexion and extension inclinations from the centerline of the body were permitted. Linear potentiometers were symmetrically attached to the slides (Bourns Instruments, Riverside, CA). These actions provided data regarding the changing distances of the handle with a 20-s sequence. An 80486 microprocessor was employed to sample the data within the range of 150 Hz. A National Instruments software; LabWindows started and stopped at 20-s sequences while presenting the captured data of the position of the limb in a time frame. The position signals were rectified with asymmetrical (triangular) Bartlett filter. The velocity time signals were obtained using the impulse of localised points by the 2-point CDA (central difference algorithm). We also applied Bartlett software prior to further rectification. The formula adapted from Kelso [
12,
21] and the smoothed time-series of position and velocity were applied to compute every component of the near continuous phase of the sequences.
ɸR=tan-{(dXR/dt)/XR})
The right wrist phase at every selected XR provides the location of the right wrist with an adjusted scale to the interval (-1 , 1) in every complete oscillation, while (dXR/dt) provides its standardized velocity at each instance. We used the same formula for the left wrist calculations of velocity and position signals. The phase relating the 2 wrists was calculated by the following formula:
φ=ɸR-ɸL
The mean Absolute Error (AE) of the relative phase was a mirror image of deviation from the intended relative phase (0° for the in-phase mode and 180° for the anti-phase mode) (precision coordination). An auditory metronome (NCH Swift Sound Tone Generator, version 2.01) provided information on the changing pace for the increasing speed of bimanual activity [
28].
Initially, the resting EEG of the subjects were recorded. This consisted of a shifted condition of simultaneous closing and opening of one eye. The EEGs were recorded at 2 electrode sites (C3, C4) corresponding to the international 10-20 system [
29]. At first, every site had an AI as the reference, then it was re-referred to linked offline ears. EEG data were collected and amplified using ProComp Infiniti system by BioGraph software. EEG signals were recorded online. BioGraph Infiniti software was installed on a Lenovo 8023 laptop. C3 and C4 for SMR power were the position of the electrode with the reference set on the right ear. The signal was changed, and the band was rectified to produce the 12 to 15 Hz SMR wave. Each participant was then oriented to the task.
A wrist extension and flexion in the left-right dimension task, required the participants to grasp 2 handles on a moving slide and move them horizontally. Each participant produced the 2 opposite phased patterns, while holding both handles. All study participants moved their hands continuously toward and then away from each other in a symmetrical manner to activate the homologous flexors and extensors wrist muscles, during the in-phase pattern. The hands moved in unison and in an iso-direction, during the anti-phase pattern. The homologous group of muscles contracted in an alternating manner in this phase. The subjects were then instructed to keep up with the pace of a metronome by conducting a complete displacement of the handle in a cycle of in-out-in time beats. The metronome facilitated the required frequency or speed gradually, from a 20-s frequency of 58 B/M. After the low-speed 20-second trial, a middle range frequency of 90 B/M was paced in the same coordination activity, before the final subsequent 152 B/M was used.
EEG sections with 6 amplitudes beyond ±100 μV were excluded from the analysis. Seven EEG data without any artifacts were taken. Hanning window (50%) was applied to these segments and thereafter Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) was used to calculate mean power spectra in the SMR (12-15 Hz). The SMR power was computed using the 12 to 15 Hz averages from C3 and C4, before transforming the natural logs.
Statistical analysis
The obtained data were analyzed by descriptive statistics and 2-way repeated measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) (2 bimanual coordination patterns ×3 speed), using SPSS. Examination of post-hoc results of importance was conducted using Bonferroni correction paired comparisons. P=0.05 was set as the significance level.
3. Results
Two-way repeated measures ANOVA results for the SMR power (Figure 1) revealed the significant main effect for speed (F2=1.35, P=0.001). Also, the primary effect of bimanual pattern was significant (F1= 7.39, P=0.001). The interaction effect of speed and bimanual pattern was significant (F2=1.27, P=0.001) (Table 1). The Bonferroni was used for more analysis of the two-way interaction of significance for bimanual pattern × speed. In anti-phase pattern, speed significantly affected all conditions (P=0.001). Through the pairwise comparisons, a major increase of SMR power were noted between slow versus medium pace (P=0.001), slow versus fast pace (P=0.001) and medium versus fast pace (P=0.001). These observations indicate that the increasing movement speed strongly influences the SMR power in the anti-phase coordination mode. In addition, pairwise comparisons between conditions reveals that the study participants produced in-phase movements with similar levels of SMR power during slow speed, average speed and fast speed (all P>0.4).
The results of two-way repeated measures ANOVA for the AE scores in the bimanual coordination mode (Figure 2) revealed a significant primary effect for bimanual patterns (F1=7.39, P=0.001) and the primary effect for speed was significant (F2=1.35, P=0.001). The interaction effect of bimanual modes and the pace was pronounced at (F2=1.27, P=0.001) (Table 2). The two-way interactional significance for speed × sensory condition was presented to the Bonferroni Test for further analysis. The in-phase mode results indicated a pronouncement with the same level of consistency across the 3 differ
ent speeds. The anti-phase results, however, indicate that the effects of speed were significant for all (P=0.001). A notable significant increase of AE results on slow versus medium pace (P=0.001), slow versus fast pace (P=0.001) and medi um versus fast pace (P=0.001) were noticed through the pairwise comparison. These observations suggest that the increasing movement speed strongly influences the performance of anti-phase coordination model.
It is concluded that SMR may be an indicator of the attentive effort, which is intense in anti-phase pattern. However, it is insufficient to compensate for the disrupting constraints that hamper the anti-phase coupling. These results indicate that SMR power is essential for a skillful performance of the bimanual task. When the speed was increased, the study participants performed anti-phase coordination tasks with higher SMR power. In other words, the SMR power is an indicator of accurate performance.
These findings are mainly supported by observations from previous studies where SMR power was essential for performance enhancement [
10,
11,
31,
33]. Furthermore, SMR is considered as an indicator for activating the cortex that related in the automatic process-related attention and skilled visuo-motor performance [
34,
35]. Additionally, SMR power that is augmenting might alter the processes associated with attention. This process occurs through the enhancement of impulse control and the power to combine critical environmental stimuli [
36].
According to the increased SMR activities, the sensorimotor cortex maintains the relaxed and focused states through reduction of the motor perception (e.g. somatosensory processing)
[5]. This finding is consistent with the principle of automaticity mentioned previously [
37]. Based on learning model of Fitz and Posner, abundant evidence suggests that peak performance is a sense of relaxation [
37]. Thus, neurofeedback training can be used in several sports and exercise sessions to improve skills. These findings are consistent with Doppelmayr and Weber’s conclusions [
11]. They revealed that augmented SMR neurofeedback training results in a significant facilitation of spatial rotation task performance. The spatial rotation ability is necessary to perform specific bimanual tasks [
11].
Neurofeedback is a tool for instruction and therapies in neurological procedures [
38]. Studies suggest that applying neurofeedback training can convert abnormal brain rhythms and frequencies to the normal ones [
39]. Neurofeedback has been formed based on the mind-body connection theory that includes training the brain to increase operating capabilities and optimal manner to experience optimal states of mind [
40].
Part of this experimental investigation was to study the function of speed and coordination of movements. Increasing speed enhanced activity of the 1800 anti-phase model, but failed to affect the 00 in-phase model. With gradual increase in speed, the stable 1800 anti-phase became weak and less stable. This occurs with growing precision and decrease in flexibility. However, the design in the 00 in-phase did not change across various frequencies. This is a replication of the findings from earlier research [
12,
21,
22,
28].
Considering the potential applications of this research, sports experts, psychologists and coaches can observe the results of this study in performance enhancements. If there is such an effect on SMR power and bimanual coordination task, we can adopt the best strategies for the development of these useful factors on motor performance and learning in bimanual coordination task. The most useful deduction from this research is that SMR neurofeedback training could enhance accuracy in bimanual coordination. The SMR neurofeedback training can be considered as a training protocol to improve bimanual coordination performance. However, applying a cross-sectional design was a limitation to our study. Future studies are recommended to examine the obtained results through longitudinal designs.
Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Funding
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Authors contributions
Conceptualization: Both authors; Methodology: Ebarhim Norouzi; Investigation: Both authors; Writing and Original Draft: Ebarhim Norouzi; Writing, Reviewing and Editing: Both authors; Funding Acquisition: Both authors; Resources: Both authors; Supervision: Mohammad Vaezmousavi.
Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgements
We thank Milad Arabloo (Sharif University of Technology) for proofreading the manuscript.
References